I know a lot of languages have some aspects that probably seem a bit strange to non-native speakers…in the case of gendered words is there a point other than “just the way its always been” that explains it a bit better?

I don’t have gendered words in my native language, and from the outside looking in I’m not sure what gendered words actually provide in terms of context? Is there more to it that I’m not quite following?

  • Lvxferre@lemmy.ml
    link
    fedilink
    arrow-up
    3
    ·
    edit-2
    11 months ago

    [Shameless comm advertisement: make sure to check !linguistics@lemmy.ml, this sort of question fits nicely there!]

    There are two main points: agreement and derivation.

    Agreement: grammatical gender gives you an easy way to keep track of which word refers to which. Consider for example the following sentence:

    • The clock fell over the glass table, and it broke.

    What does “it” refer to? It’s ambiguous, it could be either “the clock” or “the glass table” (both things are breakable). In Portuguese however the sentence is completely unambiguous due to the gender system, as the translations show:

    1. O relógio caiu sobre a mesa de vidro, e ele quebrou. // “ele” he/it = the clock
    2. O relógio caiu sobre a mesa de vidro, e ela quebrou. // “ela” she/it = the table

    It’s only one word of difference; however “ele” he/it must refer to “relógio” clock due to the gender agreement. Same deal with “ela” she/it and “mesa” table.

    Latin also shows something similar, due to the syntactically free word order. Like this:

    • puer bellam puellam amat. (boy.M.NOM pretty.F.ACC girl.F.ACC loves) = the boy loves the beautiful girl
    • puer bellus puellam amat. (boy.M.NOM pretty.M.NOM girl.F.ACC loves) = the handsome boy loves the girl

    Note how the adjective between “puer” boy and “puella” girl could theoretically refer to any of those nouns; Latin is not picky with adjective placement, as long as it’s near the noun it’s fine. However, because “puer” is a masculine word and “puella” is feminine, we know that the adjective refers to one if masculine, another if feminine. (Note: the case marks reinforce this, but they aren’t fully reliable.)

    The second aspect that I mentioned is derivation: gender gives you a quick way to create more words, without needing new roots for that. Italian examples:

    • “bambino” boy vs. “bambina” girl
    • “gatto” cat, tomcat vs. “gatta” female cat
    • “banana” banana (fruit) vs. “banano” banana plant
    • “mela” apple (fruit) vs. “melo” apple tree

    Focus on the last two lines - note how the gender system is reused to things that (from human PoV) have no sex or social gender, like trees and their fruits. This kind of extension of the derivation system is fairly common across gendered languages.


    Addressing some comments here: English does not have a grammatical gender system. It has a few words that refer to social gender and sex, but both concepts (grammatical gender and social gender) are completely distinct.

    That’s specially evident when triggering agreement in a gendered language, as English doesn’t do anything similar. Portuguese examples, again:

    • [Sentence] O Ivan é uma pessoa muito alta.
    • [Gloss, showing word gender] The.M Ivan.M is a.F person.F very tall.F
    • [Translation] Ivan is a very tall person.

    Check the adjective, “alta” tall. Even if “Ivan” refers to a man, you need to use the feminine adjective here, because it needs to agree with “pessoa” person - a feminine word. This kind of stuff happens all the time in gendered languages, but you don’t see it e.g. in English.

    • Casiraghi@feddit.it
      cake
      link
      fedilink
      arrow-up
      1
      ·
      11 months ago

      Thank you for this wonderful explanation and for taking the time to write it.

      That’s the kind of content that make this place so awesome.

  • cerement@slrpnk.net
    link
    fedilink
    arrow-up
    0
    arrow-down
    1
    ·
    11 months ago

    gendered words, plural agreement, conjugations, declensions were all forms of “parity checking” for spoken languages – ways to make sure you were accurately hearing what had been spoken

    as writing systems advanced, languages started to drop some of these forms when the written word was considered to be an “accurate” representation – ex. you can see this happening in the transition from Old English → Middle English → Modern English